Wrapping up: remaining issues
We are confident that the discussion presented above accurately represents the facts of velar palatalization as a regular phenomenon in Turkish, and as we can back it up using examples from the speech of the speaker who recorded our data. If we look hard enough, we will find that
Velars in word initial "clusters"
While dorsals occurring as the second C in a word-final consonant cluster are colored by the preceding vowel (e.g. sirc, fark in (14)), the mirror image situation is not evident when a dorsal occurs as the first C in a word-initial cluster. Native Turkish words do not have initial C clusters, but Turkish has borrowed a number of foreign words with initial sequences consisting of a stop followed by a sonorant consonant. Several examples are given in (26). Our Turkish consultant, Dr. Erdener, reports that initial consonant sequences in words such as these can be pronounced as clusters, as in (26a), but that the alternative pronunciations in (26b), in which the consonants of the borrowed sequence are pronounced with an intervening high vowel (generally [ɨ], resulting in disyllabic forms) is more natural. The examples in (13) show that only velars (no palatals) occur in initial clusters, regardless of the backness of the following vowel. For this reason, initial "clusters" cannot be treated as the "mirror image" of final clusters, even when the consonants of the clusters are identical (compare borrowed [kr] and native [rk]).
(26) |
a. | Initial Cluster | b. | Epenthetic V | ||
gri | gɨri | 'grey' | |||
kral | kɨral | 'king' | |||
grup | gɨrup | 'group' | |||
krem | kɨrem | 'cream' |
In sum, there is some question as to whether word-initial consonant sequences such as those in (13) should be considered clusters at all, since they are generally separated by a vowel on the surface (even though it is not represented orthographically). Since this vowel is the back unround vowel [ɨ], the fact that only velars appear in this environment is consistent with the distribution of velar stops as stated so far (see the discussion associated with figure (2)). For these reasons, we will not treat cases such as those in (13) as exceptions.
Palatals in velar environments
In this module, we have argued that:
This analysis predicts that:
The generalization that velars do not surface in palatal environments seems to be right - as we expect, if palatals are derived from velars in syllables containing front vowels. However, it turns out that the generalization that palatals never occur in velar environments isn't quite surface true. Clements and Sezer (1982) provide minimal and near minimal pairs showing that both palatals and velars can occur in a syllable containing the vowel [a]. Examples appear in (2).
(27) |
kar | 'snow' | car | 'profit' |
gaz | 'gas' | ɟavur | 'infidel' |
Normally, when we find two sounds in a single environment, we analyze them as belonging to phonemes that contrast in the language. Since velars are excluded from palatal environments, we still need the rule in (25) that derives palatal allophones of the velar phonemes. In this scenario, palatal allophones have two sources: underlying velars and underlying palatals. We might be forced to conclude that velars and palatals are not in complementary distribution, as we argued above, but are rather in neutralization distribution. The "phoneme-to-allophone map" under this alternative scenario is shown in (28).
(28) | /k/ | /c/ | /g/ | /ɟ/ |
[k] | [c] | [g] | [ɟ] |
However, before embracing the alternative scenario in (28), consider that things are not as straightforward as they seem. It seems that syllables with [a] is the only environment in which both palatals and velars surface, and it seems that there are not many examples of palatals occurring in the velar context. Here are some things for you to think about:
For a more extensive discussion of the disharmonic cases of palatal stops, as illustrated in (27), see Clements and Sezer (1982).